Investigating cognitive mechanisms underlying working memory manipulation across linguistic and non-linguistic domains of bilinguals
Main Article Content
Abstract
Cognitive control is a mental construct thought to be important in most cognitive tasks, including second language learning. Its functions are believed by many scholars to be mainly domain-general, with some transferability across linguistic and non-linguistic domains. Our study aims to investigate to what extent the domain-generality claim of working memory (WM) between the visual and verbal domains is true in Thai adults aged 18-36 years old. Subjects maintain and manipulate contents in the computerized WM tasks in the verbal (English, Thai, and Mixed Thai-English) and visual (kaleidoscope) domains. We hypothesized that (1) there are correlations in the WM manipulation effect of behavioral performances within the verbal domain, and (2) there are correlations in the WM manipulation effect of behavioral performances between the verbal and visual domains. Behavioral results (hit rates and reaction times) indicate significant correlations among the WM manipulation effect among the three language tasks, but not between the language and the visual tasks. Implications include that cognitive training and improvement are possible, but only within the domain. The manipulation effect can be trained across different languages using linguistic tasks, but visual tasks may not produce the desired manipulation effect in the verbal tasks. Cognitive trainings that use both linguistics and non-linguistic tasks simultaneously to train the students’ WM are recommended to achieve the manipulation effect in the language domain.
Article Details
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
References
Acheson, D. J., & MacDonald, M. C. (2009). Verbal working memory and language production: Common approaches to the serial ordering of verbal information. Psychological Bulletin, 135(1), 50–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014411
Albouy, P., Weiss, A., Baillet, S., & Zatorre, R. J. (2017). Selective entrainment of theta oscillations in the dorsal stream causally enhances auditory working memory performance. Neuron, 94(1), 193–206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.03.015
Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., Ray, J., & Kaul, S. (2013). Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81(22), 1938–1944. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000436620.33155.a4
Altarriba, J. (2006). Cognitive approaches to the study of Emotion-laden and emotion words in monolingual and bilingual memory. In A. Pavlenko (Ed.), Bilingual minds: Emotional experience, expression, and representation (pp. 232–256). Multilingual Matters. https://doi.org/10.21832/9781853598746-011
Altarriba, J., & Heredia, R. R. (2018). An introduction to bilingualism: Principles and processes (2nd ed.). Routledge.
Anderson, J. A., Mak, L., Keyvani Chahi, A., & Bialystok, E. (2018). The language and social background questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 250–263. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0867-9
Anguera, J. A., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., Faraji, F., Janowich, J., Kong, E., Larraburo, Y., Rolle, C., Johnston, E., & Gazzaley, A. (2013). Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature, 501(7465), 97–101. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12486
Atkins, P. W., & Baddeley, A. D. (1998). Working memory and distributed vocabulary learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(4), 537–552. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400010353
Avons, S. E., Wragg, C. A., Cupples, W. L., & Lovegrove, W. J. (1998). Measures of phonological short-term memory and their relationship to vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(4), 583–601. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400010377
Baars, B., & Gage, N. M. (2013). Fundamentals of cognitive neuroscience: A beginner's guide. Academic Press.
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2
Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105(1), 158–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.1.158
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1
Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2007). Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 2883–2901. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.015
Bangkok Post. (2021, 22 November). English gets raw deal. https://www.bangkokpost.com/opinion/opinion/2219235/english-gets-raw-deal
Barker, R. M., & Bialystok, E. (2019). Processing differences between monolingual and bilingual young adults on an emotion n-back task. Brain and Cognition, 134, 29–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2019.05.004
Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., Griffin, Z. M., & Hixon, J. G. (2016). Effects of age of English exposure, current input/output, and grade on bilingual language performance. Journal of Child Language, 43(3), 687–706. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000811
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45(2), 459–464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.009
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X. (2009). Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children and adults. Brain and Language, 109(2-3), 93–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.001
Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112(3), 494–500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014
Blom, E., Küntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish–Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 105–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.007
Bice, K., Yamasaki, B. L., & Prat, C. S. (2020). Bilingual language experience shapes resting-state brain rhythms. Neurobiology of Language, 1(3), 288–318. https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00014
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S. (2011). Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14(2), 256–269. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00974.x
Bouton, M. E., & Moody, E. W. (2004). Memory processes in classical conditioning. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 28(7), 663–674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.09.001
Braver, T. S., Paxton, J. L., Locke, H. S., & Barch, D. M. (2009). Flexible neural mechanisms of cognitive control within human prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(18), 7351–7356. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0808187106
Brown, G. D., & Hulme, C. (2013). Nonword repetition, STM, and word age-of-acquisition: A computational model. In G. Brown & C. Hulme (Eds.), Models of short-term memory (pp. 129–148). Psychology Press.
Brysbaert, M., & Duyck, W. (2010). Is it time to leave behind the revised Hierarchical Model of bilingual language processing after fifteen years of service? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(3), 359–371. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990344
Calkins, A. W., McMorran, K. E., Siegle, G. J., & Otto, M. W. (2015). The effects of computerized cognitive control training on community adults with depressed mood. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 43(5), 578–589. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465814000046
Carpenter, E., Rao, L., Peñaloza, C., & Kiran, S. (2020). Verbal fluency as a measure of lexical access and cognitive control in bilingual persons with aphasia. Aphasiology, 34(11), 1341–1362. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2020.1759774
Castilla, A. P., Restrepo, M. A., & Perez-Leroux, A. T. (2009). Individual differences and language interdependence: A study of sequential bilingual development in Spanish–English preschool children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 12(5), 565–580. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050802357795
Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046743
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Houghton Mifflin.
Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth and action. Elsevier.
Chein, J. M., Moore, A. B., & Conway, A. R. (2011). Domain-general mechanisms of complex working memory span. NeuroImage, 54(1), 550–559. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.067
Chenaksara, P. (2005). Needs analysis for English communication skills of Thai Airways international cabin crew. Kasetsart University.
Cheung, H. (1996). Nonword span as a unique predictor of second-language vocabulary language. Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 867–873. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.32.5.867
Cools, R., & D'Esposito, M. (2011). Inverted-U–shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), Article e113-e125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028
Cockcroft, K., Wigdorowitz, M., & Liversage, L. (2019). A multilingual advantage in the components of working memory. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(1), 15–29. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000475
Crone, E. A., Wendelken, C., Donohue, S., van Leijenhorst, L., & Bunge, S. A. (2006). Neurocognitive development of the ability to manipulate information in working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(24), 9315–9320. https://doi.org /10.1073/pnas.0510088103
D'Acierno, M. R. (1990, March). Three types of bilingualism. ERIC. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED321574
Dahlin, E., Neely, A. S., Larsson, A., Bäckman, L., & Nyberg, L. (2008). Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science, 320(5882), 1510–1512. https://doi.org /10.1126/science.1155466
Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(4), 422–433. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03214546
Darasawang, P. (2007). English language teaching and education in Thailand: A decade of change. Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
DeLuca, V., Rothman, J., Bialystok, E., & Pliatsikas, C. (2019). Redefining bilingualism as a spectrum of experiences that differentially affects brain structure and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(15), 7565–7574. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1811513116
D'Esposito, M., & Postle, B. R. (2015). The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 115–142. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W. J. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(3), 175–197. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728902003012
Diller, K. C. (1970). “Compound” and “coordinate” bilingualism: A conceptual artifact. Word, 26(2), 254–261. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1970.11435596
Dronkers, N., & Baldo, J. (2009). Encyclopedia of neuroscience. Elsevier.
Education First. (2023, November 15). EF English Proficiency Index. https://www.ef.co.th/epi/ regions/asia/thailand/
Eichenbaum, H. (2004). Hippocampus: Cognitive processes and neural representations that underlie declarative memory. Neuron, 44(1), 109–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.028
Ekman, U., Eriksson, J., Forsgren, L., Mo, S. J., Riklund, K., & Nyberg, L. (2012). Functional brain activity and presynaptic dopamine uptake in patients with Parkinson's disease and mild cognitive impairment: A cross-sectional study. The Lancet Neurology, 11(8), 679–687. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70138-2
Ellis, N. C. (1996). Sequencing in SLA: Phonological memory, chunking, and points of order. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(1), 91–126. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100014698
Emery, L., Heaven, T. J., Paxton, J. L., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Age-related changes in neural activity during performance matched working memory manipulation. Neuroimage, 42(4), 1577–1586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.021
Engel de Abreu, P. M. (2011). Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect? Memory, 19(5), 529–537. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.590504
French, R. M., & Jacquet, M. (2004). Understanding bilingual memory: Models and data. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(2), 87–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.011
Friesen, D. C., Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Proficiency and control in verbal fluency performance across the lifespan for monolinguals and bilinguals. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(3), 238–250. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.918630
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(4), 513–543. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716406060383
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28(2), 200–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90044-2
Gathercole, S. E., & Masoura, E. V. (2005). Contrasting contributions of phonological short‐term memory and long‐term knowledge to vocabulary learning in a foreign language. Memory, 13(3-4), 422–429. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210344000323
Grosjean, F., & Li, P. (2013). The psycholinguistics of bilingualism. John Wiley & Sons.
Grundy, J. G., & Timmer, K. (2017). Bilingualism and working memory capacity: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Second Language Research, 33(3), 325–340. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658316678286
Gupta, P. (2003). Examining the relationship between word learning, nonword repetition, and immediate serial recall in adults. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 56(7), 1213–1236. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980343000071
Haier, R. J. (2017). The neuroscience of intelligence. Cambridge University Press.
Hayakawa, S., Tannenbaum, D., Costa, A., Corey, J. D., & Keysar, B. (2017). Thinking more or feeling less? Explaining the foreign-language effect on moral judgment. Psychological science, 28(10), 1387–1397. https://doi.org/10.1177/095679761772094
Hayes, D. (2016). The value of learning English in Thailand and its impact on Thai: Perspectives from university students. Asia Pacific Journal of Education, 36(1), 73–91. https://doi.org/10.1080/02188791.2014.924390
Hernandez, A. E., Bodet III, J. P., Gehm, K., & Shen, S. (2021). What does a critical period for second language acquisition mean?: Reflections on Hartshorne et al.(2018). Cognition, 206, Article 104478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104478
Itthipuripat, S., Wessel, J. R., & Aron, A. R. (2013). Frontal theta is a signature of successful working memory manipulation. Experimental Brain Research, 224, 255–262. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3305-3
Jarunthawatchai, W., & Baker, W., (2024). English language education and educational policy in Thailand. In A. Moody (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of Southeast Asian Englishes (pp. 557–574). Oxford University Press.
Javor, R., & Javor, R. (2016). Bilingualism, theory of mind and perspective-taking: The effect of early bilingual exposure. Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 5(6), 143–148. https://doi.org /10.11648/j.pbs.20160506.13
Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability. Oxford University Press.
Jensen, O., & Tesche, C. D. (2002). Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working memory task. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15(8), 1395–1399. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01975.x
Jumonville, A. (2012). Encyclopedia of the sciences of learning. Reference Reviews, 26(8), 23–24. https://doi.org/10.1108/09504121211278133
Jusczyk, P. W. (1993). From general to language-specific capacities: The WRAPSA model of how speech perception develops. Journal of Phonetics, 21(1-2), 3–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31319-1
Kandel, E. R. (2007). In search of memory: The emergence of a new science of mind. WW Norton & Company.
Kaur, A., Young, D., & Kirkpatrick, R. (2016). English education policy in Thailand: Why the poor results? In R. Kirkpatrick (Ed.), English language education policy in Asia (pp. 345–361). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-22464-0_16
Kaushanskaya, M., & Prior, A. (2015). Variability in the effects of bilingualism on cognition: It is not just about cognition, it is also about bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 27–28. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000510
Klein, D., Mok, K., Chen, J. K., & Watkins, K. E. (2014). Age of language learning shapes brain structure: A cortical thickness study of bilingual and monolingual individuals. Brain and Language, 131, 20–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.014
Krause, B., & Kadosh, R. C. (2013). Can transcranial electrical stimulation improve learning difficulties in atypical brain development? A future possibility for cognitive training. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 176–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.04.001
Kray, J., & Ferdinand, N. K. (2013). How to improve cognitive control in development during childhood: Potentials and limits of cognitive interventions. Child Development Perspectives, 7(2), 121–125. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12027
Kroll, J. F., Van Hell, J. G., Tokowicz, N., & Green, D. W. (2010). The Revised Hierarchical Model: A critical review and assessment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(3), 373–381. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891000009X
Laufer, B. (1990). Why are some words more difficult than others?—Some intralexical factors that affect the learning of words. IRAL: International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 28(4), 293–308. https://doi.org/10.1515/iral.1990.28.4.293
Lehto, J. E., Juujärvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 59–80. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151003321164627
Li, P., Sepanski, S., & Zhao, X. (2006). Language history questionnaire: A web-based interface for bilingual research. Behavior Research Methods, 38(2), 202–210. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192770
Luankanokrat, S. (2011, March). Needs analysis for English communication skills of employees at HSBC in Thailand. Thammasat University. https://cdn.cocodoc.com/cocodoc-form-pdf/pdf/69080416--Needs-analysis-for-English-communication-skills-of-employees-at-HSBC-in-Thailand-Language-Institute-English-for-Careers-Research-paper-digi-library-tu-ac-.pdf
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605–621. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/20445911.2013.795574
Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114(1), 29–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.014
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Greffe, C., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Relations between vocabulary development and verbal short-term memory: The relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and item information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93(2), 95–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.07.005
Mala, D. (2018). Thai English proficiency drops. Bangkok Post. https://www.bangkokpost.com/thailand/general/1570042/thai-english-proficiency-drops
Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2007). Negative priming as a memory phenomenon. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 215(1), 35–51. https://doi.org/10.1027/0044-3409.215.1.35
Metsala, J. L. (1997). An examination of word frequency and neighborhood density in the development of spoken-word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 25(1), 47–56. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197284
Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (2013). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. Metsala & A. Walley (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Routledge.
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of neuroscience, 24(1), 167–202. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Ministry of Education. (2019, November 13). Bilingual school curriculum must overcome hurdles. The Nation. https://www.nationthailand.com/in-focus/30378314
Moradi, H. (2014). An investigation through different types of bilinguals and bilingualism. International Journal of Humanities & Social Science Studies, 1(2), 147–154. https://citeseerx.ist. psu.edu/document?repid=rep1&type=pdf&doi=7faaf99e4910126e67f7cdc28bc1124b5f682ac7#page=150
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 187–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002
Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Brill-Schuetz, K. A., Carpenter, H., & Wong, P. C. (2014). Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 56–72. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000715
Morton, J. B., Ezekiel, F., & Wilk, H. A. (2011). Cognitive control: Easy to identify but hard to define. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3(2), 212–216. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2011.01139.x
Namazi, M., & Thordardottir, E. (2010). A working memory, not bilingual advantage, in controlled attention. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(5), 597–616. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2010.488288
Papagno, C., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A. (1991). Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(3), 331–347. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90040-Q
Punthumasen, P. (2007, December). International program for teacher education: An approach to tackling problems of English education in Thailand. In The 11th UNESCO-APEID international conference reinventing higher education: Toward participatory and sustainable development (pp. 12–14). https://backoffice.onec.go.th/uploaded/Category/EngBook/ProblemEngEd13dec07-03-03-2011.pdf
Ratiu, I., & Azuma, T. (2015). Working memory capacity: Is there a bilingual advantage? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.976226
Sasisekaran, J., Smith, A., Sadagopan, N., & Weber‐Fox, C. (2010). Nonword repetition in children and adults: Effects on movement coordination. Developmental Science, 13(3), 521–532. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00911.x
Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Schabus, M., & Doppelmayr, M. (2005). Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57(2), 97–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.018
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(3), 501–518. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.501
Schmiedek, F., Lövdén, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Hundred days of cognitive training enhance broad cognitive abilities in adulthood: Findings from the COGITO study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 2, Article 27. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2010.00027
Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 45(1), 21–50. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749208401314
Singh, L. (2018). Bilingual infants demonstrate advantages in learning words in a third language. Child development, 89(4), Article e397-e413. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12852
Snodin, N., Savski, K., & Sameephet, B. (2024). English in Thailand. In A. Moody (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of Southeast Asian Englishes (pp. 311–326). Oxford University Press.
Somerville, L. H., & Casey, B. J. (2010). Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 236-241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2010.01.006
Squire, L. R., Stark, C. E., & Clark, R. E. (2004). The medial temporal lobe. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 279–306. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144130
Strobach, T., Karbach, J., & Strobach. (2016). Cognitive training. Springer.
Suthiwartnarueput, S., (2021, April 23). Sethaput Suthiwartnarueput: The Thai economy - the current state and the way forward. Bank for International Settlements. https://www.bis.org/review/r210423c.htm
Swanson, H. L. (2003). Age-related differences in learning disabled and skilled readers’ working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(1), 1–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00043-2
Thitiratsakul, T. (2024, 15 March). Education woes aren’t about funding. Thailand Development Research Institute. https://tdri.or.th/en/2024/03/education-woes-arent-about-funding/
Trakulphadetkrai, N. V. (2011). Thailand: Educational equality and quality. In C. Brock & L. Pe Symaco (Eds.), Education in South-East Asia (pp. 197–219). Symposium Books.
Tulving, E., & Schacter, D. L. (1990). Priming and human memory systems. Science, 247(4940), 301–306. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2296719
Ullman, M. T. (2001). A neurocognitive perspective on language: The declarative/procedural model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(10), 717–726. https://doi.org/10.1038/35094573
Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92(1-2), 231–270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008
Van der Linden, L., Van de Putte, E., Woumans, E., Duyck, W., & Szmalec, A. (2018). Does extreme language control training improve cognitive control? A comparison of professional interpreters, L2 teachers and monolinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 411607. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01998
Vanhove, J. (2013). The critical period hypothesis in second language acquisition: A statistical critique and a reanalysis. PloS One, 8(7), Article e69172. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069172
Van Merrienboer, J. J., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17, 147–177. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-005-3951-0
Weingarten, E., Chen, Q., McAdams, M., Yi, J., Hepler, J., & Albarracín, D. (2016). From primed concepts to action: A meta-analysis of the behavioral effects of incidentally presented words. Psychological Bulletin, 142(5), 472–497. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000030
Williams, J. N. (1999). Memory, attention, and inductive learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263199001011
Wiriyachitra, A. (2002). English language teaching and learning in Thailand in this decade. Thai TESOL Focus, 15(1), 4–9. https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/document?repid=rep1&type=pdf&doi=a0 dc5b8cd3e5cf69324df00adce0b1d3bc8965db
Zanto, T. P., & Gazzaley, A. (2013). Fronto-parietal network: Flexible hub of cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(12), 602–603. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.001